Skip to main content

Home/ TOK@ISPrague/ Group items matching "i" in title, tags, annotations or url

Group items matching
in title, tags, annotations or url

Sort By: Relevance | Date Filter: All | Bookmarks | Topics Simple Middle
Lawrence Hrubes

Dept. of Popular Culture: Banksy Was Here : The New Yorker - 0 views

  •  
    ""Why do you do what you do?" I asked. Banksy replIed, "I orIgInally set out to try and save the world, but now I'm not sure I lIke It enough.""
Lawrence Hrubes

How a Raccoon Became an Aardvark : The New Yorker - 0 views

  • In July of 2008, Dylan Breves, then a seventeen-year-old student from New York CIty, made a mundane edIt to a WIkIpedIa entry on the coatI. The coatI, a member of the raccoon famIly, Is “also known as … a BrazIlIan aardvark,” Breves wrote. He dId not cIte a source for thIs nIckname, and wIth good reason: he had Invented It. He and hIs brother had spotted several coatIs whIle on a trIp to the Iguaçu Falls, In BrazIl, where they had mIstaken them for actual aardvarks.
  • Over time, though, something strange happened: the nickname caught on. About a year later, Breves searched online for the phrase “Brazilian aardvark.” Not only was his edit still on Wikipedia, but his search brought up hundreds of other Web sites about coatis. References to the so-called “Brazilian aardvark” have since appeared in the independent, the Daily Mail, and even in a book published by the University of Chicago. Breves’s role in all this seems clear: a Google search for “Brazilian aardvark” will return no mentions before Breves made the edit, in July, 2008. The claim that the coati is known as a Brazilian aardvark still remains on its Wikipedia entry, only now it cites a 2010 article in the Telegraph as evidence.
  • This kind of feedback loop—wherein an error that appears on Wikipedia then trickles to sources that Wikipedia considers authoritative, which are in turn used as evidence for the original falsehood—is a documented phenomenon. There’s even a Wikipedia article describing it.
markfrankel18

How Birds and Babies Learn to Talk : The New Yorker - 0 views

  • Few things are harder to study than human language. The brains of living humans can only be studied indirectly, and language, unlike vision, has no analogue in the animal world. Vision scientists can study sight in monkeys using techniques like single-neuron recording. But monkeys don’t talk. However, in an article published today in Nature, a group of researchers, including myself, detail a discovery in birdsong that may help lead to a revised understanding of an important aspect of human language development. Almost five years ago, i sent a piece of fan mail to Ofer Tchernichovski, who had just published an article showing that, in just three or four generations, songbirds raised in isolation often developed songs typical of their species. He invited me to visit his lab, a cramped space stuffed with several hundred birds residing in souped-up climate-controlled refrigerators. Dina Lipkind, at the time Tchernichovski’s post-doctoral student, explained a method she had developed for teaching zebra finches two songs. (Ordinarily, a zebra finch learns only one song in its lifetime.) She had discovered that by switching the song of a tutor bird at precisely the right moment, a juvenile bird could learn a second, new song after it had mastered the first one. Thinking about bilingualism and some puzzles i had encountered in my own lab, i suggested that Lipkind’s method could be useful in casting light on the question of how a creature—any creature—learns to put linguistic elements together.
Lawrence Hrubes

BBC News - Living with the J-word - 1 views

  • Thankfully, most of this Jew-targeted hatred takes the form of verbal aggression rather than physical violence. But because many critics of israel make no distinction between citizens of the Jewish state and the worldwide Jewish community, the J-word has been the focus. You won't see "Kill israelis" scrawled on London synagogue walls. What you see on walls is "Kill the Jews", and on banners "Hitler was Right". And this brings me back to the point about the complexity of anti-Semitism today. it is always around and in the end it is focused primarily on the J-word, in the same way that another form of racism is focused on the N-word. Those on the receiving end find their lives shaped by it. Certainly my life, my sense of myself, has been shaped by the casual anti-Semitism that i have encountered for more than half a century. The first time i was called a "Jew" with malicious intent was September 1958 in the playground of Belmont Hills Elementary School, in the suburbs of Philadelphia. it came as a surprise. i was eight years old and up until that time had been living in New York City where everyone i encountered was Jewish. Until that moment, the word "Jew" had simply been one of the words and phrases - like "Mike", "son" and "114 East 90th Street" - whose meanings were slowly building up into a sense of who i was.
  • Throughout the 19th Century, "IsraelIte" or "Hebrew" or "follower of Moses" supplanted "Jew" as the polItIcally correct way to refer to the communIty. It was a process analogous to the way "black" and then "AfrIcan-AmerIcan" or "person of colour" replaced "Negro" In polIte dIscourse after the CIvIl RIghts era.
  • Thirty years later, a new word for this hatred was coined - "anti-Semitism". This was a time when race science was all the rage. Anti-Semitism avoided the connotation of pure hatred against individuals which is, after all, irrational. it focused scientifically on the supposed racial and social characteristics of a group, the Jews, without mentioning them by name. From there it was easy to start a political movement - based on scientific "facts" - to rein in a people who clearly were alien.
Lawrence Hrubes

Fertility expert: 'i can clone a human being' | Science | News | The independent - 0 views

  • "There is absolutely no doubt about it, and i may not be the one that does it, but the cloned child is coming. There is absolutely no way that it will not happen," Dr Zavos said in an interview yesterday with The independent.
  • "If we fInd out that thIs technIque does not work, I don't Intend to step on dead bodIes to achIeve somethIng because I don't have that kInd of ambItIon. My ambItIon Is to help people."
  • Dr Zavos also revealed that he has produced cloned embryos of three dead people, including a 10-year-old child called Cady, who died in a car crash. He did so after being asked by grieving relatives if he could create biological clones of their loved ones.
Lawrence Hrubes

How Science Saved Me from Pretending to Love Wine | The New Yorker - 2 views

  • was in my late forties when i finally admitted to myself that i would never love wine. As other women fake orgasms, i have faked hundreds of satisfied responses to hundreds of glasses—not a difficult feat, since my father schooled my brother and me in the vocabulary of wine from an early age. Confronted with another Bordeaux or Burgundy, i could toss around the terms i had learned at the dinner table (Pétillant! Phylloxera! Jeroboam!), then painstakingly direct the wine straight down the center of my tongue, a route that limited my palate’s exposure to what it perceived as discomfiting intensity.
markfrankel18

The Difference Between Rationality and intelligence - The New York Times - 0 views

  • The major finding was that irrationality — or what Professor Stanovich called “dysrationalia” — correlates relatively weakly with i.Q. A person with a high i.Q. is about as likely to suffer from dysrationalia as a person with a low i.Q.
  • Based on this evidence, Professor Stanovich and colleagues have introduced the concept of the rationality quotient, or R.Q. if an i.Q. test measures something like raw intellectual horsepower (abstract reasoning and verbal ability), a test of R.Q. would measure the propensity for reflective thought — stepping back from your own thinking and correcting its faulty tendencies.
eviemcconkie

Dehumanisation is a human universal - David Livingstone Smith - Aeon - 3 views

  • phenomenon of dehumanisation.
  • We dehumanise other people when we conceive of them as subhuman creatures
  • psychological essentialism’ to denote our pervasive and seemingly irrepressible tendency to essentialise categories of things.
  • ...6 more annotations...
  • People the world over segment the animal kingdom into species.
  • When we dehumanise others, we do not simply regard them as non-human. We regard them as less than human. Where does that come from?
  • Attributions of intrinsic value are intimately bound up with beliefs about moral obligation
  • we have developed methods to circumvent and neutralise our own horror at the prospect of spilling human blood.
  • You don’t have to be a monster or a madman to dehumanise others. You just have to be an ordinary human being.
  • A Midsummer Night’s Dream had no trouble understanding that though Bottom’s head looked like that of a donkey, he was really a human being ‘on the inside’, the donkeyish appearance concealing the human essence.
Lawrence Hrubes

WAYS OF SEEING HISTORY - 0 views

  •  
    "Let me first offer a simple definition of history. History is a story about the past that is both true and significant. We might quibble about the word "story" and prefer "account" or "report". Some also might be cynical about the word "true". Here i take truth to mean closest possible correspondence to what actually happened. What is truth, after all? As we answer that question, i would caution against both facile dogmatism and stylish cynicism. The human experience of truth is complex but definite."
Lawrence Hrubes

Dr. Bunsen / Coffee Experiments - 0 views

  •  
    "A few years ago, I started usIng house guests as subjects In an experIment.1 My experIment was desIgned to test what varIables In the coffee brewIng process produce a perceptIble Improvement In coffee flavor. A frequent assertIon Is that numerous varIables must be carefully consIdered to brew a good cup of coffee. I wanted to know If thIs premIse was true as humans are really good at creatIng theIr own realIty dIstortIon fIelds. "
Lawrence Hrubes

The Nocebo Effect: How We Worry Ourselves Sick | GarethCook - 0 views

  •  
    "With placebos ("i will please" in Latin), the mere expectation that treatment will help brings a diminution of symptoms, even if the patient is given a sugar pill. With nocebos ("i will harm"), dark expectations breed dark realities."
Lawrence Hrubes

Lawsuit Filed Today on Behalf of Chimpanzee Seeking Legal Personhood : The Nonhuman Rights Project - 0 views

  • The Nonhuman Rights Project is the only organization working toward actual LEGAL rights for members of species other than our own. Our mission is to change the common law status of at least some nonhuman animals from mere “things,” which lack the capacity to possess any legal right, to “persons,” who possess such fundamental rights as bodily integrity and bodily liberty, and those other legal rights to which evolving standards of morality, scientific discovery, and human experience entitle them. Our first cases were filed in 2013.
  • The legal cause of action that we are using is the common law writ of habeas corpus, through which somebody who is being held captive, for example in prison, seeks relief by having a judge call upon his captors to show cause as to why they have the right to hold him. More specifically, our suits are based on a case that was fought in England in 1772, when an American slave, James Somerset, who had been taken to London by his owner, escaped, was recaptured and was being held in chains on a ship that was about to set sail for the slave markets of Jamaica. With help from a group of abolitionist attorneys, Somerset’s godparents filed a writ of habeas corpus on Somerset’s behalf in order to challenge Somerset’s classification as a legal thing, and the case went before the Chief Justice of the Court of King’s Bench, Lord Mansfield. in what became one of the most important trials in Anglo-American history, Lord Mansfield ruled that Somerset was not a piece of property, but instead a legal person, and he set him free.
markfrankel18

Do You Believe in Magic? interview with Dr. Paul Offit - CSi - 0 views

  • I thInk there Is somethIng to be saId for the placebo response, whIch Is unfortunately named. I thInk when people hear the word “placebo,” they assume that It's dIsmIssIve, trIvIalIzIng, that It's just all In my head and that It's not real. I thInk that In fact, the placebo response can be a physIologIcal response. I thInk belIevIng that you are about to be helped In some way goes a long way to beIng helped In some way.
  • If you belIeve that human anatomy has nothIng to do wIth rIvers In ChIna or days of the year, then there's nothIng accurate about acupuncture. Indeed, If you look at people who benefIt from acupuncture, It doesn't matter where you put the needles. In fact, It doesn't even matter whether you put the needles under the skIn! Even these retractable needles seem to work, so‑called “acupressure.” That Is InterestIng!
markfrankel18

Science isn't Broken | FiveThirtyEight - 0 views

  • If we’re goIng to rely on scIence as a means for reachIng the truth — and It’s stIll the best tool we have — It’s Important that we understand and respect just how dIffIcult It Is to get a rIgorous result
  • Scientists’ overreliance on p-values has led at least one journal to decide it has had enough of them. in February, Basic and Applied Social Psychology announced that it will no longer publish p-values.
  • P-hacking and similar types of manipulations often arise from human biases. “You can do it in unconscious ways — i’ve done it in unconscious ways,” Simonsohn said. “You really believe your hypothesis and you get the data and there’s ambiguity about how to analyze it.” When the first analysis you try doesn’t spit out the result you want, you keep trying until you find one that does.
  • ...4 more annotations...
  • From 2001 to 2009, the number of retractions issued in the scientific literature rose tenfold. it remains a matter of debate whether that’s because misconduct is increasing or is just easier to root out.
  • Science isn’t broken, nor is it untrustworthy. it’s just more difficult than most of us realize. We can apply more scrutiny to study designs and require more careful statistics and analytic methods, but that’s only a partial solution. To make science more reliable, we need to adjust our expectations of it.
  • Science is not a magic wand that turns everything it touches to truth. instead, “science operates as a procedure of uncertainty reduction,” said Nosek, of the Center for Open Science. “The goal is to get less wrong over time.”
  • Some of these biases are helpful, at least to a point. Take, for instance, naive realism — the idea that whatever belief you hold, you believe it because it’s true. This mindset is almost essential for doing science, quantum mechanics researcher Seth Lloyd of MiT told me. “You have to believe that whatever you’re working on right now is the solution to give you the energy and passion you need to work.” But hypotheses are usually incorrect, and when results overturn a beloved idea, a researcher must learn from the experience and keep, as Lloyd described it, “the hopeful notion that, ‘OK, maybe that idea wasn’t right, but this next one will be.’”
Lawrence Hrubes

ImagInIng the LIves of Others - NYTImes.com - 0 views

  • What could be more exhilarating than experiencing the world through the perspective of another person? in “Remembrance of Things Past,” Marcel Proust’s narrator says that the only true voyage of discovery is not to visit other lands but “to possess other eyes, to behold the universe through the eyes of another, of a hundred others, to behold the hundred universes that each of them beholds.” This is one of the central projects of the humanities; it’s certainly part of the pleasure we get from art and literature.
  • People are often highly confident in their ability to see things as others do, but their attempts are typically barely better than chance. Other studies find that people who are instructed to take the perspectives of others tend to do worse, not better, at judging their thoughts and emotions.
  • There are certain limits, however, to how far we can go. The philosopher Laurie Paul, in her book “Transformative Experience,” argues that it’s impossible to actually imagine what it would be like to have certain deeply significant experiences, such as becoming a parent, changing your religion or fighting a war. The same lack of access applies to our understanding of others. if i can’t know what it would be like for me to fight in a war, how can i expect to understand what it was like for someone else to have fought in a war? if i can’t understand what it would be like to become poor, how can i know what it’s like for someone else to be poor?
markfrankel18

What it feels like to be the last generation to remember life before the internet - Quartz - 0 views

  • Harris takes a different path from those that have come before. instead of a broad investigation into the effects on constant connectivity on human behaviour, Harris looks at a very specific demographic: people born before 1985, or the very opposite of the “millennial” demographic coveted by advertisers and targeted by new media outlets.
  • It was neIther better nor worse than the world we lIve In today. LIke technology, It just was.
  • That means being able to notice things like the reduction of interactions to numbers, and how that translates into quantifications of human worth. “i think it has to do with this notion of online accountability. That is, noticing that you actually count seems to be related to a sense of self worth,” he says over the phone from Toronto, where he is based. “So it’s like if a tweet gets retweeted a couple of hundred times, that must mean that my thoughts are worthy. if my Facebook photo is ‘liked,’ that must mean i am good looking. One of the things that concerns me about a media diet that is overly online, is that we lose the ability to decide for ourselves what we think about who we are.” +
  • ...1 more annotation...
  • Instead of wonderIng what should I do, we wonder what dId I mIss.
Lawrence Hrubes

BBC News - A Point of View: The upside of losing one's memory - 0 views

  • Reading the scientific research, i learn that the brain starts deteriorating from the mid-20s onwards. By our 40s and 50s, it's well under way. The changes include a drop in brain volume, loss of myelin integrity, cortical thinning, impaired receptor binding and signalling, and altered concentrations of various brain metabolites. The accumulation of neuro-fibrillary tangles, something we associate with Alzheimer's, also happens in normal ageing.
  • You may be asking what i am worrying about. i'm not yet 50, after all. i don't have Alzheimer's or Parkinson's or any reason to be worried about those diseases. My favourite novelist, Penelope Fitzgerald, was nearly 80 when her last and best book, The Blue Flower, was published. The philosopher Mary Midgley is still sharp as a razor, and she'll be 96 next birthday. She didn't even write a book till she was past 50. The psychoanalyst Hedda Bolgar was still seeing patients at the age of 102.
  • Last week, my colleague listened to his father, behind a drawn hospital curtain, doing an Alzheimer's diagnostic test. He said that the trickiest part was when they read out a list of words but instead of asking you to repeat them they moved on to some numerical questions before asking you to list the words a moment later. He was petrified to find that he couldn't do it himself.
  • ...1 more annotation...
  • If I was lookIng for advIce about anythIng other than whIch smart phone to buy, I'd generally ask someone aged 80- rather than an 18-year-old. It's lIke when you take a photograph - there's the shutter speed to consIder, but there's also the depth of fIeld. PerspectIve matters.
Lawrence Hrubes

Teaching Doubt - The New Yorker - 0 views

  • “Non-overlapping magisteria” has a nice ring to it. The problem is that there are many religious claims that not only “overlap” with empirical data but are incompatible with it. As a scientist who also spends a fair amount of time in the public arena, if i am asked if our understanding of the Big Bang conflicts with the idea of a six-thousand-year-old universe, i face a choice: i can betray my scientific values, or encourage that person to doubt his or her own beliefs. More often than you might think, teaching science is inseparable from teaching doubt.
  • Doubt about one’s most cherished beliefs is, of course, central to science: the physicist Richard Feynman stressed that the easiest person to fool is oneself. But doubt is also important to non-scientists. it’s good to be skeptical, especially about ideas you learn from perceived authority figures. Recent studies even suggest that being taught to doubt at a young age could make people better lifelong learners. That, in turn, means that doubters—people who base their views on evidence, rather than faith—are likely to be better citizens.
  • Science class isn’t the only place where students can learn to be skeptical. A provocative novel that presents a completely foreign world view, or a history lesson exploring the vastly different mores of the past, can push you to skeptically reassess your inherited view of the universe. But science is a place where such confrontation is explicit and accessible. it didn’t take more than a simple experiment for Galileo to overturn the wisdom of Aristotle. informed doubt is the very essence of science.
  • ...1 more annotation...
  • Some teachers shy away from confronting religious beliefs because they worry that planting the seeds of doubt will cause some students to question or abandon their own faith or the faith of their parents. But is that really such a bad thing? it offers some young people the chance to escape the guilt imposed upon them simply for questioning what they’re told. Last year, i received an e-mail from a twenty-seven-year-old man who is now studying in the United States after growing up in Saudi Arabia. His father was executed by family members after converting to Christianity. He says that it’s learning about science that has finally liberated him from the spectre of religious fundamentalism.
markfrankel18

PQED: How should people respond to open-carry gun-rights activists? - 0 views

  • The difficulty of knowing other people’s intent is a classic philosophical problem. it is epistemological in that it involves the limits of our knowledge. We can’t really know what anyone else hopes to do, and sometimes, because of the subconscious and self-deception, we don’t ever know what our own true intent is. it is also an example of the problem of other minds. We can never really enter into the perspective of any other person nor can we ever really know what they think (or even if they think). We are discrete individuals and communication is unreliable. My point: the political and economic realities of running from gun activists is, yet again, founded on classic philosophical issues, and when we take positions on issues of the day, we are really taking positions philosophically. The gun-rights activists think that their intent is obvious and that everyone knows what they hope to do. They believe their minds are transparent. But this is because they are all extreme narcissists. it baffles them that we don’t all know exactly what they are thinking. it shocks them that we don’t know that Jim is a good guy, and that Sally would never murder anyone. But they are wrong. We don’t know them and we don’t know how they think. The only thing that makes us notice them at all is that they have guns and truthfully, that’s why they carry them in the first place. They want to be celebrities, heroes, and the centers of attention.
markfrankel18

With Naming Rights, 'Perpetuity' Doesn't Always Mean Forever - NYTimes.com - 1 views

  • I asked hIm, How long Is ‘In perpetuIty’?”“For you, 50 years,” Mr. de Montebello, the museum dIrector, replIed.
  • “Perpetuity is usually a matter of negotiation now,” said William D. Zabel, a lawyer representing the Fisher family, who had threatened to sue on their behalf 12 years ago when Lincoln Center considered changing the name at that time without its permission.“it’s like in ‘Alice in Wonderland’: ‘When i use a word, it means just what i choose it to mean — neither more nor less,’ ” he said.
‹ Previous 21 - 40 of 204 Next › Last »
Showing 20 items per page